Finance suffers from a replication crisis and it is well known that once a market anomaly has been described in the academic literature it tends to disappear in financial markets. But what if factor performance never disappeared? What if it is just hiding behind a corner? That at least is what a study from CUNY says.
Typically, when academics describe a market anomaly like the value, momentum, or size effect, they form portfolios based on some past observations, then let the portfolio run for a month and do the entire exercise again. The hope is that this monthly rolling process will systematically create outperformance over the market as time passes.
Unfortunately, with the proliferation of smart beta investing and other quantitative methods in finance, factors that are described in the literature are increasingly exploited by systematic investors to the extent that these factors seem to disappear. The chart below shows the average performance of 260 factors described in the literature in the US stock market over different time periods. Looking at the green bars shows that since 2009, the average outperformance of these factors has almost halved from about 0.6% to 0.35%.
Factor performance in US equity markets
Source: Dong and Yang (2023)
Something interesting happens if one looks at the average factor performance in months 2 to 24 as is done with the purple diamonds in the chart. Essentially, instead of reconstituting factor portfolios every month, the researchers let each factor portfolio run for two years and then did the exercise again. Leaving portfolios untouched shows no decay of outperformance after 2009.
The key to understanding what is going on here seems to be the behaviour of retail investors. Retail investors display some well-known biases, and they tend to trade with some anomalies (for example, retail traders trade with experts when it comes to momentum stocks) but they tend to trade against some other anomalies. For example, retail investors prefer growth stocks over value stocks, low quality, unprofitable stocks over high quality, profitable companies or they prefer lottery stocks with an unlikely but very large positive return vs. more likely small negative returns. In total, of the 260 anomalies examined in the study, retail investors trade against some 60% of the anomalies and with about 40% of the anomalies.
Look at the outperformance of anomalies where retail traders trade on the same side as the professionals. The decline in outperformance is much more pronounced and even over the next 24 months, the outperformance completely disappears in the years after 2009.
Factor performance in US equity markets when retail investors trade with anomaly returns
Source: Dong and Yang (2023)
Compare this with the anomalies where retail investors trade against the anomaly.
Factor performance in US equity markets when retail investors trade against anomaly returns
Source: Dong and Yang (2023)
Here, the size of the outperformance remains more stable in the first month and even increases in the months 2 to 24.
This indicates that it is the growing influence of retail traders that influences the performance of systematic factors. When retail investors trade with professionals, they quickly destroy the factor performance because the combined flows of systematic traders and retail investors lead to a rapid adjustment of share prices. But when retail investors trade against systematic factors, the flows of retail investors push against the factor, share prices correct much slower and mispricing survives for longer. And that means that patient factor investors can still exploit these factors if they leave their factor portfolios untouched for longer.
Replication...The entire scientific world is a bit fishy there (you gotta love them psychologists...):
'A 2016 survey by Nature on 1,576 researchers who took a brief online questionnaire on reproducibility found that more than 70% of researchers have tried and failed to reproduce another scientist's experiment results (including 87% of chemists, 77% of biologists, 69% of physicists and engineers, 67% of medical researchers, 64% of earth and environmental scientists, and 62% of all others), and more than half have failed to reproduce their own experiments.
But fewer than 20% had been contacted by another researcher unable to reproduce their work. The survey found that fewer than 31% of researchers believe that failure to reproduce results means that the original result is probably wrong, although 52% agree that a significant replication crisis exists. Most researchers said they still trust the published literature.[5][90]
In 2010, Fanelli (2010)[91] found that 𝟗𝟏.𝟓% 𝐨𝐟 𝐩𝐬𝐲𝐜𝐡𝐢𝐚𝐭𝐫𝐲/𝐩𝐬𝐲𝐜𝐡𝐨𝐥𝐨𝐠𝐲 𝐬𝐭𝐮𝐝𝐢𝐞𝐬 𝐜𝐨𝐧𝐟𝐢𝐫𝐦𝐞𝐝 𝐭𝐡𝐞 𝐞𝐟𝐟𝐞𝐜𝐭𝐬 𝐭𝐡𝐞𝐲 𝐰𝐞𝐫𝐞 𝐥𝐨𝐨𝐤𝐢𝐧𝐠 𝐟𝐨𝐫, and concluded that the odds of this happening (a positive result) was around five times higher than in fields such as astronomy or geosciences. Fanelli argued that this is because researchers in "softer" sciences have fewer constraints to their conscious and unconscious biases.'
https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Replication_crisis
Economics seems to lag a bit when it comes to researching itself but studies hint at about 30% of studies that are 'wanting'.
Which still makes economists perform 2 x better than climate scientists. Bring on the energy 'transition'!